Saturday, April 27, 2013

Hata Yoga elevates your consciousness

from newindianexpress.com




28th April 2013 12:00 AM
Hata Yoga is a preparatory process of yoga. You can also explore Hata Yoga in ways that take you beyond certain limitations, but fundamentally, it is a physical preparation—preparing the body for a higher possibility. If we want to do Kriya Yoga, we always prepare people with Hata Yoga because without the body being prepared, it will not be able to take higher dimensions of energy. It will break. It is just like if your pipe is not ready and you pump in too much force, it is bound to burst. So Hata Yoga is preparation of the pipe. We prepare the pipe as a channel, so that when you do Kriya Yoga, your system is ready to take it.
Understanding the mechanics of the body, creating a certain atmosphere, and then using the body or body postures to drive your energy in specific directions is what Hata Yoga or yogasanas are about. Hata Yoga is not exercise. Asana means a posture. If I sit in one way, it is one asana. If I sit in another way, it is another asana. So innumerable asanas are possible. Out of these innumerable postures that the body can take, 84 fundamental postures have been identified as yogasanas.
There are other dimensions to this, but to put it in the simplest way, just by observing the way somebody is sitting, you almost know what is happening with them, if you have known them long enough. If you know yourself, if you have observed yourself, if you are angry, you will sit one way; if you are happy, you sit another way; if you aredepressed, you sit another way. For every different level of consciousness or mental and emotional situation that you go through, your body naturally tends to take certain postures. The converse of this is the science of asanas. If you consciously get your body into different postures, you can also elevate your consciousness.
I am incapable of being depressed; otherwise I would be depressed looking at the way Hata Yoga is being practiced around the world and people thinking that is what it is. The practice as you see it, the mechanics of it, is simply of the body. You have to breathe life into it, otherwise it will not become alive. This is why, in the traditions, there has been so much stress on a live Guru—to make it alive. The yogic system is a subtle manipulation of your system to allow it to rise to a different level. Yoga means, that which allows you to attain to your higher nature. Every asana, every mudra, every way of breathing—everything —is focused towards this.
After 20 years of yoga entering the West and becoming popular, medical professionals are now coming forth and making studies and saying, “Yoga has benefits”. Right now the number of people practicing yoga—even the simplest Hata Yoga—is growing in a big way simply because the scientific community is slowly beginning to recognise the depth and dimension of what it is. Though it is taught in frivolous ways, still thehealth benefits of it are undeniable around the world. But if improper, distorted kind of yoga spreads, in 10 to 15 years’ time, scientific studies will clearly come out and tell you in how many ways it is harmful to human beings, and that will be the downfall.
If Hata Yoga is taught in a proper atmosphere with a certain sense of humility and inclusiveness about the whole process, it is a very fantastic process of shaping your system into a fantastic vessel, a fabulous device to receive the Divine. There are certain dimensions of Hata Yoga which are almost absent in the world today. It is there in some places, but generally in the known places, it is absent. I would like to bring those dimensions. It is a very powerful way of living. Power, not over somebody else; power to access life.
The writer is a world-renowned spiritual leader.
www.ishafoundation.org

Friday, April 26, 2013

Babies’ Brains: When Does Consciousness Emerge?

from time




sb10066477a-002 (1)a
KRISTIN DUVALL / GETTY IMAGES
Finding the point at which babies’ reactions change from being purely reflexive to reflecting more intention is leading researches to focus on the first glimmers of conscious thought in infants as young as five months old.
“We can prove that the same neuromarkers of consciousness found in adults can be found in babies as early as five months of age,” says lead author Sid Kouider, a researcher at the Ecole Normale Superieure in Paris, of his new study on the earliest signs of conscious thought in infants that was published in Science.
To look for consciousness in babies too young to talk, the authors took advantage of research on visual perception, which showed that the brain processes a great deal of visual information before any of it reaches a level of conscious awareness. EEG signals, which are measured by placing electrodes on the head, can clearly distinguish between visual data that is consciously seen and that which is simply taken in by the brain. These signals show a major change when a person first becomes consciously aware of an object that has previously received only subliminal attention.
“There are 2 stages of perceptual processing,” explains Kouider, “The first stage is basically activation of neurons in the sensory cortex. Just a little visual stimulation— even if you can’t see it consciously— is going to activate [this brain region].” The brain still shows electrical activity on an EEG, for example, even if images or words flash by so quickly that they aren’t consciously perceived. (This information registers somewhere in the brain, however, because such “subliminal” data can affect responses to later tasks).
The second stage, which can be reported verbally by adults, comes with a different signal and is essentially either “all”— when you can see it — or “none,” if the object isn’t visible at all, indicating a conscious level of attention and processing.
To better understand how, and when these different levels of perception might be engaged in babies, the researchers placed EEG caps on 30 five-month-olds, 29 one-year-olds and 21 15-month-old toddlers and had them look at images of faces to determine whether they would generate the signals associated with the second, more intentional type of processing. And indeed, Kouider says they found that the babies’ brains traced the same signature of consciousness as adult brains.
“What changes basically is that the neural signal of consciousness we observe is weaker at five months,” he says, “It’s less stable and it’s much slower.” Indeed, a five month old must see an image for four times longer than adult would to show a signal of conscious visual awareness— and this takes three times longer for 12 and 15 month-olds.
“It’s intriguing,” says Lise Eliot, associate professor of neuroscience at the Rosalind Franklin University of Medicine and Science in Chicago and author of What’s Going On In There?, which examines infant brain development. Eliot notes that while previous research suggested that there might be signals of consciousness in infants, “This is a much more elegant, carefully controlled study and they do have a lot of the adult parallels to relate this to.”
Kouider and his colleagues plan to look at even younger babies next, as they try to determine at what age the signal first appears. The results could have implications for expanding our understanding what consciousness is and how it develops in the brain. “It would be neat if you could use infants to figure out what consciousness is because it suddenly appears at some age,” says Eliot.
Kouider says he’s not surprised that consciousness is not as well developed in infants as it is in adults because it requires interplay between the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and sensory brain regions— and during the first three years of life, the connections between those regions aren’t very robust since the PFC and the wiring of those connections are still developing. “If [a perception is] not going to be subliminal, it has to involve the PFC,” he says, “We know from anatomical studies that the PFC is underdeveloped until the second year of life. It’s not nonexistent, it’s just not fully functional.”
The research also has more practical implications. Ideally, the infant studies would enable scientists to trace a trajectory of how consciousness generates. “You can start to use this method very early to basically try to check whether there is normal or abnormal development,” Kouider says. “We know that autistic children can have trouble being aware of faces and you could imagine this kind of method to diagnose early on whether someone is reacting in a normal way to objects or faces.”
So while the youngest babies may not be conscious of much, they can provide valuable information about how conscious thought emerges, and how it develops over time. And with that understanding, we may learn a great deal about what it means to be aware.



Read more: http://healthland.time.com/2013/04/26/babys-first-consciousness/#ixzz2RalLlgz2

Thursday, April 25, 2013

Consciousness After Death: Strange Tales From the Frontiers of Resuscitation Medicine

from wired.com




Image: Emilio Labrador/Flickr
Sam Parnia practices resuscitation medine. In other words, he helps bring people back from the dead — and some return with stories. Their tales could help save lives, and even challenge traditional scientific ideas about the nature of consciousness.
“The evidence we have so far is that human consciousness does not become annihilated,” said Parnia, a doctor at Stony Brook University Hospital and director of the school’s resuscitation research program. “It continues for a few hours after death, albeit in a hibernated state we cannot see from the outside.”
Resuscitation medicine grew out of the mid-twentieth century discovery of CPR, the medical procedure by which hearts that have stopped beating are revived. Originally effective for a few minutes after cardiac arrest, advances in CPR have pushed that time to a half-hour or more.
New techniques promise to even further extend the boundary between life and death. At the same time, experiences reported by resuscitated people sometimes defy what’s thought to be possible. They claim to have seen and heard things, though activity in their brains appears to have stopped.
It sounds supernatural, and if their memories are accurate and their brains really have stopped, it’s neurologically inexplicable, at least with what’s now known. Parnia, leader of the Human Consciousness Project’s AWARE study, which documents after-death experiences in 25 hospitals across North America and Europe, is studying the phenomenon scientifically.
Parnia discusses his work in the new book Erasing Death: The Science That Is Rewriting the Boundaries Between Life and Death. Wired talked to Parnia about resuscitation and the nature of consciousness.
Wired: In the book you say that death is not a moment in time, but a process. What do you mean by that?
Sam Parnia: There’s a point used to define death: Your heart stops beating, your brain shuts down. The moment of cardiac arrest. Until fifty years ago, when CPR was developed, when you reached this point, you couldn’t come back. That led to the perception that death is completely irreversible.
But if I were to die this instant, the cells inside my body wouldn’t have died yet. It takes time for cells to die after they’re deprived of oxygen. It doesn’t happen instantly. We have a longer period of time than people perceive. We know now that when you become a corpse, when the doctor declares you dead, there’s still a possibility, from a biological and medical perspective, of death being reversed.
Of course, if someone dies and you leave them alone long enough, the cells become damaged. There’s going to be a time when you can’t bring them back. But nobody knows exactly when that moment is. It might not just be in tens of minutes, but in over an hour. Death is really a process.
'The idea that electrochemical processes in the brain lead to consciousness may no longer be correct.'
Wired: How can people be brought back from death?
Parnia: Death is, essentially, the same as a stroke, and that’s especially true for the brain. A stroke is some process that stops blood flow from getting into the brain. Whether it’s because the heart stopped pumping, or there was a clot that stopped blood flow, the cells don’t care.
Brain cells can be viable for up to eight hours after blood flow stops. If doctors can learn to manipulate processes going on in cells, and slow down the rate at which cells die, we could go backand fix the problem that caused a person to die, then re-start the heart and bring them back. In a sense, death could become reversible for conditions for which treatments become available.
If someone dies of a heart attack, for example, and it can be fixed, then in principle we can protect the brain, make sure it doesn’t experience permanent cellular death, and re-start the heart. If someone dies of cancer, though, and that particular cancer is untreatable, then it’s futile.
Wired: Are you talking about bringing people to life days or weeks or even years after they’ve died?
Parnia: No. This is not cryogenics. When you die, most of your cell death occurs through apoptosis, or programmed cell death. If your body is cold, the chemical reactions underlying apoptosis are slower. Making the body cold slows the rate at which cells decay. But we’re talking about chilling, not freezing. The process of freezing will damage cells.
Wired: You also study near-death experiences, but you have a different term for it: After-death experience.
Parnia: I decided that we should study what people have experienced when they’ve gone beyond cardiac arrest. I found that 10 percent of patients who survived cardiac arrests report these incredible accounts of seeing things.
When I looked at the cardiac arrest literature, it became clear that it’s after the heart stops and blood flow into the brain ceases. There’s no blood flow into the brain, no activity, about 10 seconds after the heart stops. When doctors start to do CPR, they still can’t get enough blood into the brain. It remains flatlined. That’s the physiology of people who’ve died or are receiving CPR.
Not just my study, but four others, all demonstrated the same thing: People have memories and recollections. Combined with anecdotal reports from all over the world, from people who see things accurately and remember them, it suggests this needs to be studied in more detail.
Wired: One of the first after-death accounts in your book involves Joe Tiralosi, who was resuscitated 40 minutes after his heart stopped. Can you tell me more about him?
Parnia: I wasn’t involved in his care when he arrived at the hospital, but I know his doctors well. We’d been working with the emergency room to make sure they knew the importance of starting to cool people down. When Tiralosi arrived, they cooled him, which helped preserve his brain cells. They found vessels blocked in his heart. That’s now treatable. By doing CPR and cooling him down, the doctors managed to fix him and ensure that he didn’t have brain damage.
When Tiralosi woke up, he told nurses that he had a profound experience and wanted to talk about it. That’s how we met. He told me that he felt incredibly peaceful, and saw this perfect being, full of love and compassion. This is not uncommon.
People tend to interpret what they see based on their background: A Hindu describes a Hindu god, an atheist doesn’t see a Hindu god or a Christian god, but some being. Different cultures see the same thing, but their interpretation depends on what they believe.
Wired: What can we learn from the fact that people report seeing the same thing?
Parnia: At the very least, it tells us that there’s this unique experience that humans have when they go through death. It’s universal. It’s described by children as young as three. And it tells us that we should not be afraid of death.
Wired: How do we know after-death experiences happen when people think they do? Maybe people misremember thoughts from just before death, or just after regaining consciousness.
Parnia: That’s a very important question. Do these memories occur when a person is truly flatlined and had no brain activity, as science suggests? Or when they’re beginning to wake up, but are still unconscious?
The point that goes against the experiences happening afterwards, or before the brain shut down, is that many people describe very specific details of what happened to them during cardiac arrest. They describe conversations people had, clothes people wore, events that went on 10 or 20 minutes into resuscitation. That is not compatible with brain activity.
It may be that some people receive better-quality resuscitation, and that — though there’s no evidence to support it — they did have brain activity. Or it could indicate that human consciousness, the psyche, the soul, the self, continued to function.
Wired: Couldn’t the experiences just reflect some extremely subtle type of brain activity?
Parnia: When you die, there’s no blood flow going into your brain. If it goes below a certain level, you can’t have electrical activity. It takes a lot of imagination to think there’s somehow a hidden area of your brain that comes into action when everything else isn’t working.
These observations raise a question about our current concept of how brain and mind interact. The historical idea is that electrochemical processes in the brain lead to consciousness. That may no longer be correct, because we can demonstrate that those processes don’t go on after death.
There may be something in the brain we haven’t discovered that accounts for consciousness, or it may be that consciousness is a separate entity from the brain.
Electrical activity in the brain as a heart enters cardiac arrest. Image:Kano et al./Resuscitation
Wired: This seems to verge on  supernatural explanations of consciousness.
Parnia: Throughout history, we try to explain things the best we can with the tools of science. But most open-minded and objective scientists recognize that we have limitations. Just because something is inexplicable with our current science doesn’t make it superstitious or wrong. When people discovered electromagnetism, forces that couldn’t then be seen or measured, a lot of scientists made fun of it.
Scientists have come to believe that the self is brain cell processes, but there’s never been an experiment to show how cells in the brain could possibly lead to human thought. If you look at a brain cell under a microscope, and I tell you, “this brain cell thinks I’m hungry,” that’s impossible.
It could be that, like electromagnetism, the human psyche and consciousness are a very subtle type of force that interacts with the brain, but are not necessarily produced by the brain. The jury is still out.
Wired: But what about all the fMRI brain imaging studies of thoughts and feelings? Or experiments in which scientists can tell what someone is seeing, or what they’re dreaming, by looking at brain activity?
Parnia: All the evidence we have shows an association between certain parts of the brain and certain mental processes. But it’s a chicken and egg question: Does cellular activity produce the mind, or does the mind produce cellular activity?
Some people have tried to conclude that what we observe indicates that cells produce thought: here’s a picture of depression, here’s a picture of happiness. But this is simply an association, not a causation. If you accept that theory, there should be no reports of people hearing or seeing things after activity in their brain has stopped. If people can have consciousness, maybe that raises the possibility that our theories are premature.
Wired: What comes next in your own research?
Parnia: In terms of resuscitation, we’re trying to non-invasively measure what happens in the brain, in real-time, using a special sensor that allows us to detect any impending danger and intervene before extensive damage is done.
On the question of consciousness, I’m interested in understanding the brain-based modulators of consciousness. What helps a person become conscious or unconscious? How can we manipulate that to help people who look like they’re unconscious? And I’m studying how consciousness can be present in people who’ve gone beyond the threshold of death. All we can say now is that the data suggests that consciousness is not annihilated.
Pages: 1 2 View All

Wednesday, April 24, 2013

The Brain of a Bomber: Did Damage Caused By Boxing Play a Role in the Boston Bombings?

from  time




Boston Marathon Bombing Suspect Tamerlan Tsarnaev Boxing Pictures
GLENN DEPRIEST / GETTY IMAGES
Tamerlan Tsarnaev is telling no tales. The older of the two brothers who committed the Boston Marathon bombings was likely the one who planned the attack, but when he died in a shootout with police just days after the blasts, his thoughts and motivations vanished with him. But the brain that was home to his angry mind remains, and in this case that may mean something.
Tsarnaev was an amateur boxer who won the New England Golden Gloves competition as recently as 2009 and 2010. That speaks to a young man with a healthy sense of discipline and focus, and if he had a violent streak, it was violence well-channeled. But his sport of choice suggests the possibility of something else too: traumatic brain injury. As the National Football League and other pro sports increasingly reckon with the early dementiamental health issuessuicides and even criminal behavior of former players, the risk of what’s known as chronic traumatic encephalopathy (CTE), is becoming clear. Roughly 4,000 former NFL players and 2,000 of their spouses are currently suing the league, claiming that the perils of head injuries were never explained to them and, indeed, that the players were pushed to get back on the field even when it was clear that they had suffered concussions.
It was inevitable, then that questions would be raised about  whether Tsarnaev’s brain may have been similarly traumatized during the years he boxed, and if there had indeed been damage, did that spark his murderous behavior? The answer is a likely yes to the first part and a likely no to the second.
Boxers are perhaps the best-studied victims of CTE, with the consequences of consistent trauma to the head described initially as “punch drunk,” but emerging as CTE in the 1950s, says Dr. Robert Stern, cofounder of the Boston University Center for the Study of Traumatic Encephalopathy. The term “better describes a neurodegenerative disease caused, at least in part, by repetitive brain trauma,” he says.
The presence of CTE can only be confirmed postmortem, by looking for tau proteins in the brain—produced when neural connections stretch and show signs of wear. Whether the presence of those proteins, however, play any role in behavior is less clear. In general, older people with CTE exhibit problems with memory, attention span and the ability to learn new material—all of the things we associate with Alzheimer’s disease and other forms of dementia. They may also exhibit impulsiveness, anger, situational explosiveness and paranoia, which are familiar as well to families of people suffering from dementia. In younger people, the predominance of the symptoms is frequently reversed: outbursts and lack of impulse control come first, with an underlying loss of cognitive abilities eventually following.
And that’s the problem with looking for CTE in Tarnaev. Authorities haven’t revealed if they plan to examine his brain, and unless they have taken some necessary steps to preserve it for study they may have lost that option already. But it may not matter. “The Boston bombing was a planned attack,” says Dr. Robert Cantu, also of Boston University and co-director of the Encephalopathy Center. “There were lots of explosive devices put together in a very premeditated way. There was no flipping out here, no impulsiveness. That’s not what you see with CTE in younger people.” That doesn’t mean Tsarnaev didn’t have brain trauma related to his boxing. “I think he did,” Cantu says. It’s just that it may not have caused his criminal behavior.
That points to the difficulty of establishing any link between the condition of a brain and actions that may or may not result from it. Cantu points to the case of the late pro wrestler Chris Benoit, who killed his wife and son and then himself in 2007. When his brain was studied after he died, it showed signs of CTE—but here too it might have had little to do with his murderous behavior. “In Benoit’s case the behavior was again premeditated. It took place slowly, over the course of a weekend. He even sedated his son first so he wouldn’t suffer,” Cantu says. Criminally pathological? Certainly. But triggered by CTE? Probably not.
For now, there is no cure for CTE and the best treatment is prevention, which sports leagues from Pop Warner football on up are beginning to address with rules changes and more stringent medical monitoring of players. As my colleague Sean Gregory reported in January, investigators at UCLA have developed a still-experimental technique to detect CTE in the brains of living patients, using positron emission tomography (PET) scans and a radioactive dye that clings to tau proteins. Early diagnosis, if it becomes routine, could be used to keep already ill people away from contact sports and prevent their condition from being exacerbated by more hits. They might also be enrolled in treatment and therapy programs that would slow the onset of what for now are inevitable symptoms.
While none of this likely would have deterred Tsarnaev, it might be used to diagnose other people at risk of explosive CTE-related violence and stop them before they act out. By treating a single person’s wounded brain, doctors could one day save uncounted other lives.


Read more: http://healthland.time.com/2013/04/23/cte/#ixzz2RRtEQ4Kj

Monday, April 22, 2013

A Bear’s Brain Surgery, Babies’ Consciousness and More

from nytimes




Matt Hunt
In Laos, an Asiatic black bear with hydrocephalus — water on the brain — became the first bear to have a brain operation.
  • FACEBOOK
  • TWITTER
  • GOOGLE+
  • SAVE
  • E-MAIL
  • SHARE
  • PRINT
  • REPRINTS
You’re up in space — weightless — with a wet washcloth. What will happen when you wring it out?
NASA
Chris Hadfield wrung out a wet washcloth while aboard the International Space Station for an experiment designed by two 10th graders in Nova Scotia.
Even on Friday, when everyone was riveted to the manhunt in Boston, anonline video from the International Space Station about that question was a suspenseful and intriguing diversion. What happened to the water in microgravity turned out to be “much more interesting than what happens here on earth,” as The Los Angeles Times put it.
Before we give you the answer, here’s the context: Canada’s equivalent of NASA held a contest for schoolchildren, asking them to design a simple experiment that the Canadian astronaut aboard the space station could perform using particular household items, office supplies and the like. The list of nearly 50 items included dental floss, nylon running shorts, ketchup, mustard and a magnifying glass.
Two 10th-grade girls from Nova Scotia, together with their science teacher, came up with the wet washcloth idea, chosen from nearly 100 entries. So last Tuesday, the commander of the space station, Chris Hadfield, a veteran spacewalker, recorded a video in which, with a microphone bobbing near his mouth, he unfurled a white washcloth, squirted drinking water onto it, then twisted the saturated cloth. In what looked like a magic trick, the water began surrounding the washcloth in a thick layer, then migrated outward.
“It’s becoming a tube of water,” Commander Hadfield said, staring intently at the cloth over his signature mustache. “It wrings out of the cloth onto my hands.”
Because of the surface tension of the water, he explained, the fluid ran along the exterior of the fabric and onto him, where it stuck, “almost like you had Jell-O on your hand.” He wiped his wet hands and left the washcloth, still twisted, dangling in place. “Great experiment, worked perfectly,” he said. The 10th graders had predicted the outcome spot on. “Their hypothesis that the water would not drip in microgravity but rather remain on the washcloth was proven correct,” as the Canadian Space Agency put it in a news release. “In the absence of gravity to pull the water down, it took a shake or a quick squeeze from Commander Hadfield for the washcloth to release the water.” Or, as one person commented on the video on YouTube, “Everything is cooler in space.”
Developments
Veterinary Medicine
Ursine Neurosurgery
In Laos, an Asiatic black bear with hydrocephalus — water on the brain, which causes fierce headaches — underwent laparoscopic surgery, becoming the first bear ever to have a brain operation, National Geographic reported. “Rescued as a cub, Champa stood out from the start,” the magazine said. “She had a protruding forehead and had trouble socializing with the other bears at the sanctuary. Over time, her growth slowed, her behavior became more erratic, and her vision faded.” Euthanizing Champa was not considered an option in the largely Buddhist country, so a British veterinary surgeon,Romain Pizzi, who describes his specialty as “minimally invasive ‘keyhole’ surgery of wildlife,” operated on her, just as he has done on seals, reindeer and jaguars. Six weeks post-op, Champa looked perkier, her caretakers said; she had gained weight and grown chummier with other bears.
Health
Warding Off Breast Cancer
It has long been a thorny topic: Should healthy women take tamoxifen and raloxifene, powerful drugs that can reduce their risk of breast cancer but can also cause life-threatening strokes and blood clots? Some resolution arrived last week, when the United States Preventive Services Task Force — a gold standard among medical experts —recommended that doctors offer to prescribe the drugs for women with higher than average risk of breast cancer and low risks of blood clots and strokes. The drugs work by blocking the effects of estrogen in breast tissue, where it can stimulate the growth of some tumors. “These medicines may have benefits for women who are at increased risk of breast cancer and at low risk for harms from the medicines,” the task force said in a fact sheet, but “women who are not at increased risk for breast cancer should not use tamoxifen or raloxifene to reduce their risk for breast cancer.”
Infant Development
What Do Babies Know?
Five-month-old infants can’t tell you what they are seeing and thinking (though their mothers can, of course). But if you attach electrode caps to their heads — which is harmless, though it looks disturbing — you can get a glimpse of their brain activity. AFrench neuroscientist did this, plopping infants in their parents’ laps and measuring the neural patterns generated when the babies viewed a face flashed on a screen. In results that seem oddly reassuring, the babies showed the same brain reactions that adults do — a “two-phase pattern that would indicate consciousness,” as the Web site LiveScience put it — but it took them longer to get there. “In 5-month-olds, it took 1.3 seconds for the second flurry of brain activity to show up,” LiveScience noted. “In adults, the timing is closer to three-tenths of a second.” The small study, which was published in Science, adds weight to the argument that very young infants are conscious of their surroundings, meaning that their reactions are not just reflexive.
Geology
Earth Shakings
Two bits of seismic activity news came up last week — totally separate, both startling. One was that the horrific fertilizer plant explosion in West, Tex., which caused at least 14 deaths and left a wide swath of devastation, was so powerful that the United States Geological Survey registered it as a 2.1-magnitude earthquake. And the seismic impact ofHurricane Sandy last October may have been the same or larger: Keith D. Koper, a geophysicist at the University of Utah, and a colleague reported that waves pounding the seafloor generated “microseisms” that were picked up around the country. “There is no magnitude scale for the microseisms generated by Sandy, but Koper says they range from roughly 2 to 3 on a quake magnitude scale,” ScienceDaily, a news Web site, reported. “The conversion is difficult because earthquakes pack a quick punch, while storms unleash their energy for many hours.”